Coming to estimates obtained at the area level (
Table 4), compared to others in the Arts and Humanities, the impact of articles is much less sensitive to the age of cited articles. This is an area where the coverage of WoS and the intensity of publications are low compared to other areas (Archambault et al.,
2006; Hicks,
1999). Furthermore, the pace of discoveries and technological advances is notoriously slower than in STEMM. The impact of articles in the Arts and Humanities is much more sensitive than in other areas to the open-access features of publications. Interpretation of these results is complex. A possible explanation is that the countries’ contribution to this area has a larger geographical scope than that of STEMM. Free access to literature in less affluent countries might partly explain this marked difference. Mathematics is another area with peculiar features. Within STEMM, it shows the lowest intensity of publications and collaboration work (D’Angelo & Abramo,
2015), the lowest citation counts for journals, papers, and authors (Adler et al.,
2008), and the lowest citation accrual speed (Wang,
2013). Compared to other areas, the associations to the impact of the number of countries in the byline and the reference list length are markedly higher, and vice versa, that of the manuscript length is appreciably lower. Evidently, in mathematics, numbers count more than words; alongside cooperative work involving different countries and works based on more sources contribute more than other features to the manuscript’s impact. The average quality of authors’ past scientific publications is confirmed to be a meaningful and relevant factor with no exception; the positive association ranges from a minimum in Clinical Medicine (0.157) to a maximum in Mathematics (0.299). On the contrary, “linguistic advantage” shows several exceptions: in particular, in Arts and Humanities and Law, political and social sciences, the coefficient is positive, although not significant. One possible interpretation is that being an English mother tongue does not help in quantitative discipline studies, unlike others. The same holds for gender influence. Anyway, in the other eight areas, the negative association with the impact of female co-authors remains statistically significant. The interpretation here is not straightforward. While there are certain areas of the globe where gender homophily and citation sexism are more pronounced (Wang et al.,
2021), they should represent only a slight share of overall world citations. A more plausible explanation is that female scholars, in general, hold lower social capital than males (Rhoten & Pfirman,
2007), as shown by their smaller international collaboration networks (Abramo et al.,
2013; Larivière et al.,
2011; Uhly et al.,
2015), which is probably due to their lower inclination to travel abroad for conferences and research, with some exceptions in specific fields (Zimmer et al.,
2006). Their comparatively lower social relationships at the international level might contribute to lower citation rates, with all others being equal. It is worth noting that there is a slight variation of the R squared across areas (min 0.22 in Psychology, max 0.31 in Chemistry); thus, the predictive ability of the considered explanatory variables is similar.