Research Paper

Delayed recognition: recent developments and a proposal to study this phenomenon as a fuzzy concept

  • Ronald Rousseau†
Expand
  • University of Antwerp, Faculty of Social Sciences, B-2020 Antwerp, Belgium d KU Leuven, Facultair Onderzoekscentrum ECOOM, Naamsestraat 61, Leuven B-3000, Belgium
Corresponding author: Ronald Rousseau (Email: ; ).

Online published: 2010-10-28

Copyright

Open Access

Abstract

Purpose: New developments in the study of delayed recognition are discussed.

Design/methodology/approach: Based on these new developments a method is proposed to characterize delayed recognition as a fuzzy concept.

Findings: A benchmark value of 0.333 corresponding with linear growth is obtained. Moreover, a case is discovered in which an expert found delayed recognition several years before citation analysis could discover this phenomenon.

Research limitations: As all citation studies also this one is database dependent.Practical implications: Delayed recognition is turned into a fuzzy concept.

Originality/value: The article presents a new way of studying delayed recognition.

Cite this article

Ronald Rousseau† . Delayed recognition: recent developments and a proposal to study this phenomenon as a fuzzy concept[J]. Journal of Data and Information Science, 2018 , 3(3) : 1 -13 . DOI: 10.2478/jdis-2018-0011

1 Introduction

A publication suffering from delayed recognition is a publication that received very little attention shortly after publication, but received recognition later. Stephen Cole proposed to use citations as a proxy for recognition (Cole, 1970). Although recognition can be given in many ways—receiving tenure is another important way in which scientists are recognized for their achievements—collecting received citations is the most practiced way to operationalize the notion of delayed recognition. This contribution is not meant as a review of the topic, but we concentrate on a few recent developments. Yet, among the many papers written by colleagues on delayed recognition we single out for mention: (Bornmann et al., 2018; Burrell, 2005; Du & Wu, 2016; El Aichouchi & Gorry, 2018; Garfield, 1980; Glänzel et al., 2003; Ke et al., 2015; Li & Ye, 2012; van Raan, 2004, 2015, 2017).
In this short paper we will discuss three aspects: naming of the phenomenon, recent methods based on a cumulative citation curve and re-interpretation of delayed recognition as a fuzzy concept.

2 Naming of the concept

The concept of delayed recognition in relation to persons or articles has also been described as premature discovery, suffering from Mendel’s syndrome, being late bloomers or being ahead of one’s time. Mendel’s work on the rules of heredity is often considered as the prototype case. Yet, Mendel’s work was not totally unknown before the 20th century as mentioned by Garfield (1970), giving reference to Zirkle (1964).
In an article published in 2004, Ton van Raan proposed the name “sleeping beauty” for an article suffering delayed recognition (van Raan, 2004). This catchy term took on immediately: on June 3, 2018 van Raan’s article had already received 176 citations in the Web of Science (WoS). When the value, importance or usefulness of such a “sleeping beauty” is finally recognized in another article, denoted here as P, serving as a wake-up call for the scientific community (leading to general recognition of the “sleeping beauty”), article P is referred to as the Prince, continuing the metaphor of the story of the Sleeping Beauty. The act of “awakening” the sleeping beauty is then sometimes referred to as “the kiss.”
Sugimoto and Mostafa (2018) recalled that, in the context of sleeping beauties, Braun et al. (2010, p. 198) discussed the “ideal couple” and further sexualized the metaphor by discussing male and female dominance and “absolute superiority”: a measurement of the relative citations achieved by the prince and the sleeping beauty. Finally, they introduced the notion of chastity of sleeping beauties, in terms of the number of articles that awoke the dormant article and mentioned the possible unfaithful behavior of princes. Clearly a form of sexualization of citation trajectories has been—and is still—going on.
It is clear that these types of metaphors, continuing with “brave girls” for articles which are immediately recognized (Ye & Bornmann, 2018) have the tendency to become more and more gender-loaded. For this reason Sugimoto & Mostafa (2018) wrote an editorial, decrying this “clear violation of sociocultural norms”. They made a plea to future authors that the use of any such terms, despite connections to historical roots in the literature, should be avoided. As a consequence they stated that JASIST’s author guidelines will be adapted to make this policy explicit and clear.
As a reaction Hu et al. (2018) proposed the metaphor of gender-neutral terms “hibernator” and “awakener” to replace the terms “sleeping beauty” and “prince”. It is, of course, an open question if any metaphor is really useful.

3 A new approach to determine articles with delayed recognition based on a cumulative citation curve

The approach proposed by Ke et al. (2015)
Although being a sleeping beauty sounds like a yes/no situation, it is clear that delayed recognition is not a clear-cut phenomenon and a sleeping beauty in the eyes of one person may not be one in the eyes of a colleague. A similar observation holds in relation to the citation database used for collecting citations. To solve this problem Ke et al. (2015) turned delayed recognition into a time-dependent continuous phenomenon by defining a beauty coefficient at time T, denoted as B(T). In the next section we return to the fact that these authors turned a yes/no phenomenon into a continuous one. Now we focus on the practical way in which they did this. Let c(t) denote the yearly citation curve of an article, i.e., c(t) is the number of citations received in year t. The publication year is year t = 0 and t takes values between
0 and T. Let cm > 0 be the maximum yearly number of received citations by this article, for which we assume that it happened in year tm, with 0 < tmT. The line connecting (0,c(0)) and the peak (tm,cm) = (tm,c(tm)), which is referred to as the recognition line, is denoted as y(t), and has equation:
Then,
The numerator of a term in B(T) is equal to the—signed—difference between the recognition line and the citation value. As the denominator of this term is equal to the number of citations (unless this number is zero, in which case the denominator is 1) each term in the sum determining B(T) is a relative value.
If c(t) has a concave trajectory then B(T) is negative.
If c(t) is approximately linear then B(T) is (close to) zero.
If c(t) is convex then B(T) is positive.
If now each term in the sum determining B(T) is non-negative, then the following properties hold.
-) All else staying the same, B(T) is increasing when cm increases.
-) All else staying the same, B(T) decreases when c(t), with t fixed and different from 0 or tm, increases as the numerator decreases and the denominator increases.
Using cumulative citation curves
In recent papers Du & Wu (2017, 2018) note some disadvantages of the definition proposed by Ke et al. (2015), the most important one being the high importance given to the peak. They claim that the determination of the B-value works well for publications that after discovery have huge numbers of citations every year, but for publications with fewer citations, it may lead to some unwanted results. They, moreover, consider the role of the denominator in the original definition as just a way to avoid division by zero.
For these reasons these authors propose a different approach, not based on the citation curve, c(t), but on the cumulative citation curve . Using a variation on the Du and Wu approach based on the cumulative citation curve we will propose a description of delayed recognition as a fuzzy phenomenon.

4 Delayed recognition as a fuzzy phenomenon

Now we propose a framework to study delayed recognition of an article at a given moment in time, say T. More precisely, we consider the question: does this article suffers delayed recognition or has it in the past (while now it, perhaps, behaves like a normal article, already receiving a declining number of citations).
Studying this question we consider three aspects: “delayed,” “recognition” and fuzzy membership.
When it comes to the “delayed” part, this implies that one must wait a certain period before one may say that there is a delay. In this study we wait at least ten years (see further for details), but further investigations are needed to study the influence of this starting time. Does it matter if one starts investigations 10 years after publication or is 15 or 20 years better?
Next we come to the “recognition” part. We propose to concentrate on the 1% most cited publications in the same publication year as the publication under investigation. A choice must further be made to include all publication types in this 1% or only normal articles (or normal articles and reviews). We think that
here all choices are valid, i.e., have some scientific value, but the choice must be stated clearly.
Finally we come to the most difficult part: constructing a framework to come to a fuzzy membership value. This value, between zero and one, must in a meaningful way express to which extent an article can be said to belong to the fuzzy set of publications with delayed recognition. This membership function, as calculated at time T, is denoted as DR(T). If an article is not “recognized”, i.e. it does not belong to the 1% most cited, it is not ahead of its time and its DR(T) value is set equal to 0 for any T. Our approach is based on ideas from Ke et al. (2015) and Du and Wu (2018).
To the best of our knowledge Ke et al. (2015) were the first to state that suffering delayed recognition is not a yes-no situation. They introduced a parameter-free measure that quantifies the extent to which a specific paper can be considered to suffer delayed recognition. Papers with citations growing linearly with time have
B = 0. B is non-positive for papers whose citation trajectory is a concave function of time and positive for papers with a convex citation curve. Du and Wu (2017, 2018) proposed a similar measure, but based on the cumulative citation curve.
We will calculate a partial membership function, denoted as K(t), for each time t between 10 and T. The final DR(T) value is then equal to:
The use of the maximum function in formula (3) avoids that the DR coefficient diminishes over time, which is against the definition of the concept of delayed recognition: once an article is accepted to have suffered delayed recognition this cannot be undone.
When determining K(t) for given t, we define C(n) equal to the cumulative number of received citations at the beginning of year n (where year 0 is the publication year) and hence C(0) = 0; c(1) = C(1) denotes the number of citations received during the publication year. If C(t) = 0 then K(t) is set equal to 0. If now C(t) ≠ 0, we consider the line y(n) connecting the origin (0,0) with the point (t, C(t)). This line, which we call the recognition line at time t, has equation
Now we calculate the sum of the differences in each n, 0 ≤ nt, between the line y(n) and the cumulative citation curve C(n). This sum is denoted as S(t).
The largest possible value of S(t) occurs when all C(n) are zero except C(t).
This happens if the publication receives its first citation in the year t. Yet, we are not interested in that year, but just use this value as a reference. For this case, . Finally K(t) is defined as the ratio of the observed S(t) value over the largest possible one:
leading to a value between -1 and +1.
K(t) is negative if C(n) is always situated above the line y(n) and certainly positive when C(n) is always situated under this line. Yet, K(t) may also be positive when parts of C(n) are above the recognition line y(n).
We note that if an article receives its first citation in year 10 and is ‘recognized’ then, based on equation (3), its DR(T) value is equal to one, for all T ≥ 10.

5 Examples

Theoretical examples
If the cumulative citation curve is everywhere concave then K(t) is always negative and DR(T) = 0 for every T. Similarly, if c(n) is constant: c(n) = a > 0, then C(n) = a*n and the recognition line has equation y(n) = a*n. Clearly, also here DR(T) = 0, agreeing with the fact that there is no delayed recognition.
If citations grow linearly in time, then c(n) = b*n (b > 0), C(n) = b*n(n+1)/2 and C(T) = b*T(T+1)/2 and hence . Consequently, DR(T) = 1/3 (The calculation is included in the appendix). This result is different from the one obtained by using Ke et al.’s B. Their B-value is zero, although citations grow with time, indicating a delay in recognition. We further remark that this value for linear growth can be used as a kind of benchmark when comparing to other citation curves. Recall that linear growth in citation corresponds to quadratic growth in cumulative citations as illustrated in Figure 1 for b = 0.5.
Some real-world examples
In this contribution we provide three examples, leaving more investigations to further research.
As a real-world example we begin with Romans’ article (Romans, 1986), an article studied by van Raan (2004). This article got its first citation in 1995 (n = 10), followed by 11 more in 1996. Since then it kept on receiving citations with a peak in 1999, in which year it received 32 citations. The WoS includes 520,862 publications of article type published in 1986. Among these, the article ranked 5209 received 229 citations. As Romans’ article received 374 citations it belongs to the top 1% most-cited (data collected on June 5 2018). Figure 2 shows the cumulative citation curve, the recognition line for the year 2017, when K(2017) is 0.241 and the recognition line in the year 1996. Its DR value is equal to 1.0 obtained in the year 1996, which is the first year for which we perform a calculation. Hence DR(T) = 1 for all T ≥ 10. We recall that these calculations are performed at the beginning of the year: n = 0 correspond to the publication year and citations received during the publication year are associated with the year n=1.
Figure 1. Quadratic cumulative growth corresponding to linear growth.
Next we consider Leaky et al. (1964). This article has been studied as a sleeping beauty in (Tobias, 1996). The WoS contains 127,018 publications of article type published in 1964. Among these the article ranked 1271 received 242 citations. As Leakey et al. (1964) received 348 citations it belongs to the top 1% most-cited (data collected on June 5 2018). Its DR-value is 0.225 which is obtained in the latest year studied, namely 2017. This value is smaller than the benchmark value of 0.333 obtained for linear growth. Its lowest K(n) value is -0.084, which was obtained for n = 39 (the year 2003). Note that this value was obtained several years after Tobias (1996) had declared this article to be a sleeping beauty! Figure 3 shows the cumulative citation curve, its final recognition line and the situation in the year 2003, when the recognition line was situated under the citation curve.
Figure 2. Cumulative citation curve of Romans (1986) and two recognition lines.
Figure 3. Cumulative citation curve of Leakey et al. (1964) and two recognition lines.
Table 1 K-values for Leakey et al. (1964).
Year t K(t) Year t K(t) Year t K(t) Year t K(t)
1974 0.014 1985 -0.019 1996 -0.032 2007 -0.007
1975 0.020 1986 -0.055 1997 -0.032 2008 0.027
1976 0.005 1987 -0.053 1998 -0.061 2009 0.061
1977 -0.006 1988 -0.011 1999 -0.059 2010 0.060
1978 -0.013 1989 0.013 2000 -0.057 2011 0.085
1979 -0.035 1990 0.016 2001 -0.055 2012 0.102
1980 -0.036 1991 -0.015 2002 -0.074 2013 0.145
1981 -0.023 1992 -0.026 2003 -0.084 2014 0.151
1982 0.023 1993 0.005 2004 -0.041 2015 0.164
1983 0.003 1994 -0.022 2005 -0.017 2016 0.194
1984 -0.004 1995 -0.023 2006 -0.006 2017 0.225
This leads us to question Tobias’ paper (1996). What did he claim? It is important to know that, actually, Tobias was a co-author of the Leakey et al. (1964) paper. In his paper from 1996 he described how their findings were not accepted by their colleagues, but that step by step the original objections against their findings and corresponding theory fell away and, in his words, by 1984 their findings were accepted. This happened twenty years after their publication and hence, these findings were—rightly—described as a premature discovery. Honesty forces us to include that even today the exact position of Homo habilis in the development of the genus Homo is not yet convincingly determined.
The citation curve does not show any sign of this observation. We think this illustrates the very important fact that using citations is just an operationalization and experts may, rightly, have other opinions. We note that this article and Romans’ are also under-cited influential and hence citation chimeras in the sense of (Hu & Rousseau, 2018). This term refers to the fact that these articles are exceptional in terms of received citations and in terms of second-generation citations.
Finally, we consider one of our own articles, namely (Otte & Rousseau, 2002). Again, we first check if it belongs to the top 1% most-cited articles. The WoS contains 813,472 publications of article type published in 2002. Among these the article ranked 8,135 received 280 citations. As Otte and Rousseau (2002) received 368 citations it belongs to the top 1% most-cited (data collected on June 5, 2018). The K(t)-values first decline somewhat before they start increasing. Only in the latest year the maximum is reached: DR(2017) = 0.523. The cumulative citation curve and the recognition line for 2017 are shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4. Cumulative citation curve of Otte and Rousseau (2002).
Table 2 K-values for Otte and Rousseau (2002).
Year t K(t)
2011 0.515
2012 0.480
2013 0.476
2014 0.455
2015 0.464
2016 0.477
2017 0.523

6 Conclusion

We reviewed recent developments related to the study of delayed recognition, leading to the idea to consider delayed recognition as a fuzzy concept. We proposed a method to obtain fuzzy membership values. One of the requirements for suffering delayed recognition, is that the article must belong to the 1% most-cited ones. This means that at most 1% of the articles under consideration have a non-zero fuzzy membership value, and probably much less than 1%. The value 0.333 for linear growth in citations can be considered a benchmark for comparisons.
Besides proper hibernators (sleeping beauties) who have a long period with no or few citations, articles suffering delayed recognition may have a convex cumulative citation curve, such as in the case of linear growth in citations. Examples of these two types are shown in this contribution: Romans (1986) being a proper hibernator and Leakey et al. (1964) and Otte & Rousseau (2002) being examples of the second type.
We made the important observation that using citations to study delayed recognition is just a—convenient—operationalization of the concept, but that experts may agree on delayed recognition long before this is shown by citations. This is illustrated by the case of Leakey et al. (1964). This leads to the question: How good (adequate) is citation analysis for detecting premature discoveries?
As this contribution is just a feasibility study, many questions are left unanswered, such as:
(1) What are typical values for membership functions?
(2) Wouldn’t it be better to use normalized citation scores instead of absolute ones as done here? If so, how to normalize: with respect to the database, with respect to the field, or both (Bornmann et al., 2018)?
(3) Can this framework, by focusing on negative values and years immediately after the publication year, also be used for characterizing early recognition (flash-in-the-pan)? If so, how?
These questions are left as topics for further research.
Finally we mention the obvious limitation: as all citation studies also this one is database dependent.

Appendix: Calculation of DR(T) in the case of a linear increase in citations.

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

[1]
Bornmann L., Ye A.Y., & Ye F.Y. (2018). Identifying “hot papers” and papers with “delayed recognition” in large-scale datasets by using dynamically normalized citation impact scores. Scientometrics, .

[2]
Braun T., Glänzel W., & Schubert A. (2010). On Sleeping Beauties, Princes and other tales of citation distributions .. Research Evaluation, 19(3), 195-202.It was demonstrated that the phenomenon of ‘Sleeping Beauties’ in scientific literature is a rather useful and instructive model in studying the mechanisms of scientific information flow. On the basis of the several idiosyncratic behavioral patterns in the citation histories of Sleeping Beauties and Princes a sketchy typology could be drawn. Furthermore, the process of ‘induced citations’ (i.e.citations adopted from the reference list of a subsequent paper) has been studied in detail. It was found that citations, as a rule, are induced by papers published in higher impact factor journals than the original. Copyright , Beech Tree Publishing.

DOI

[3]
Burrell Q.L. (2005). Are “Sleeping Beauties” to be expected? Scientometrics, 65(3), 381-389.A paper that is little cited ('sleeps') for a long period of time and then becomes much cited ('is awakened'), has been termed by van Raan (2004) a 'Sleeping Beauty', or a paper that was 'ahead of its time'. The inference is that the importance of the paper was not initially recognised, only later was it (re)discovered. On the other hand, much theoretical work in informetrics views the citation process as being purely random - modelled by an appropriate stochastic process. From this point of view, the 'awakening' could simply be a matter of chance without necessarily saying anything about the worth of the paper. The question therefore arises as to whether such awakenings can be explained or expected purely by the random nature of the model or whether they are so unlikely that an alternative explanation should be sought. In this note we express the notion of a Sleeping Beauty in terms of a well-known stochastic model and seek to answer this question, at least in general terms.

DOI

[4]
Cole S. (1970). Professional standing and the reception of scientific discoveries. American Journal of Sociology, 76, 286-306.The Matthew Effect occurs when scientists receive differential recognition for a particular scientific contribution depending on their location in the stratification system. Merton originally introduced the concept to explain the allocation of credit among authors of multiple discoveries or collaborators. In this paper the concept is generalized to apply to all scientific work. If the Matthew Effect were to operate, the reception of papers of equal quality should be influenced by the location of their authors in the stratification system. To test this gypothesis, data are drawn from several studies of similar design. In each study we control for the number of citations paper received at time 2. This enables us to look at groups of papers that were judged to be roughly equal in quality at time 2. We then see whether there were any differences in the reception of these papers at time 1 depending upon various aspects of the author's location in the stratification system. All the data indicate that assessed quality of papers at time 2 is a more important determinant of paper's initial reception than any of the stratification variables. However, the speed of diffusion of papers of equal quality is influenced by the reputation of the author based on past work that is being heavily utilized at the time of a new discovery. The Matthew Effect also operates for those scientists located at prestigious points of the social system of science. All other stratification variables, including eminence as measured by receipt of awards, did not influence the speed of diffusion. Data are presented that indicate that top papers written by high-ranking scientists are no more likely to be widely diffused early than are top papers by low-ranking scientists. However, lesser quality papers by high-ranking scientists receive greater attention than papers of equal quality by low-ranking scientists. The Matthew Effect also serves to focus attention on the work of little-known men who collaborate with scientists of high repute and to increase retroactively the visibility of the early work of scientists who to on the greater fame. A discussion is included of the relevance of these data for the study of resistance to scientific discoveries.

DOI

[5]
Du J . &Wu Y. (2016). A bibliometric framework for identifying “Princes” who wake up the “Sleeping Beauty” in challenge-type scientific discoveries. Journal of Data and Information Science, 1(1), 50-68.

[6]
Du J. &Wu, Y.(2017). A parameter-free index for identifying under-cited sleeping beauties in science. Proceedings of ISSI 2017—The 16th International Conference on Scientometrics and Informetrics. Wuhan University, China, (148-157).

[7]
Du , J. &Wu , Y.(2018). A parameter-free index for identifying under-cited sleeping beauties in science. Scientometrics

[8]
El Aichouchi, A., &Gorry, P. (2018). Paul Hagenmüller’s contribution to solid state chemistry:A scientometric analysis. Journal of Solid State Chemistry, 262(June), 156-163.

[9]
Garfield E. (1970). Would Mendel’s work have been ignored if the Science Citation Index was available 100 years ago? Current Contents. Reprinted in:Essays of an Information Scientist, Vol. 1, 1962-1973 (p. 69-70) 1973, Philadelphia, ISI.

[10]
Garfield E. (1980). Premature discovery or delayed recognition—Why? Current Contents, 21. Reprinted in: Essays of an Information Scientist, Vol.4, 1979-1980(p.488-493) 1980 Philadelphia, ISI.

[11]
Glänzel W., Schlemmer B., & Thijs B. (2003). Better late than never? On the chance to become highly cited only beyond the standard bibliometric time horizon. Scientometrics, 58(3)

571-586.

[12]
Hu X.J., Zhang Y.N., Hu X.Y., & Rousseau R. (2018). Hibernators, their awakeners and the roles of subsequent authoritative citers. Malaysian Journal of Library & Information Science, 23(1), 103-113.

[13]
Hu , X.J. &Rousseau R. (2018). Do citation chimeras exist? The case of under-cited influential articles suffering delayed recognition. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology (submitted).

[14]
Ke Q., Ferrara E., Radicchi F., & Flammini A. (2015). Defining and identifying sleeping beauties in science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(24), 7426-7431.Abstract: A Sleeping Beauty (SB) in science refers to a paper whose importance is not recognized for several years after publication. Its citation history exhibits a long hibernation period followed by a sudden spike of popularity. Previous studies suggest a relative scarcity of SBs. The reliability of this conclusion is, however, heavily dependent on identification methods based on arbitrary threshold parameters for sleeping time and number of citations, applied to small or monodisciplinary bibliographic datasets. Here we present a systematic, large-scale, and multidisciplinary analysis of the SB phenomenon in science. We introduce a parameter-free measure that quantifies the extent to which a specific paper can be considered an SB. We apply our method to 22 million scientific papers published in all disciplines of natural and social sciences over a time span longer than a century. Our results reveal that the SB phenomenon is not exceptional. There is a continuous spectrum of delayed recognition where both the hibernation period and the awakening intensity are taken into account. Although many cases of SBs can be identified by looking at monodisciplinary bibliographic data, the SB phenomenon becomes much more apparent with the analysis of multidisciplinary datasets, where we can observe many examples of papers achieving delayed yet exceptional importance in disciplines different from those where they were originally published. Our analysis emphasizes a complex feature of citation dynamics that so far has received little attention, and also provides empirical evidence against the use of short-term citation metrics in the quantification of scientific impact.

DOI PMID

[15]
Leakey L.S.B., Tobias P. V., & Napier J. R. (1964). A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature, 202(4927), 7-9.Nature. 1964 Apr 4;202:7-9.

DOI PMID

[16]
Li J., &Ye, F.Y. (2012). The phenomenon of all-elements-sleeping-beauties in scientific literature. Scientometrics, 92(3), 795-799.AbstractThe phenomenon of all-elements-sleeping-beauties in science is revealed by four special cases. The ‘sleeping beauties’ prick their fingers on the ‘spindles’ so that they fall into sleep then are awakened by their ‘princes’. The authors speculate that the phenomenon could happen in scientific literatures with high quality.

DOI

[17]
Otte, E. &Rousseau ,R.(2002). Social network analysis: A powerful strategy, also for the information sciences. Journal of Information Science, 28(6), 441-453.

[18]
Romans L.J.(1986). Massive N = 2a supergravity in ten dimensions. Physics Letters B, 169(4), 374-380.A new version of the non-chiral N = 2a supergravity in ten dimensions is obtained, in which the two-index tensor field of the theory “eats” the single vector field and acquires a mass in a Higgs-type mechanism. The new theory, although it contains no fundamental vectors, bears many formal resemblances to gauged supergravities (in particular, the recently constructed F(4) theory in six dimensions). The scalar potential has no extrema, but nevertheless the classical equations of motion admit a wide variety of spontaneous compactifications, many to four dimensions.

DOI

[19]
Sugimoto C.R., &Mostafa , J. (2018). A note of concern and context: On careful use of terminologies. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 69(3), 347-348.The phenomenon of all-elements-sleeping-beauties in science is revealed by four special cases. The ‘sleeping beauties’ prick their fingers on the ‘spindles’ so that they fall into sleep then are awakened by their ‘princes’. The authors speculate that the phenomenon could happen in scientific literatures with high quality.

DOI

[20]
Tobias P.V.(1996). Premature discoveries in science with especial reference to “Australopithecus” and “Homo Habilis”. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 140(1), 49-64.

DOI

[21]
van Raan,A.F.J. (2004). Sleeping Beauties in science. Scientometrics, 59(3), 467-472.<a name="Abs1"></a>A &#8216;Sleeping Beauty in Science&#8217; is a publication that goes unnoticed (&#8216;sleeps&#8217;) for a long time and then, almost suddenly, attracts a lot of attention (&#8216;is awakened by a prince&#8217;). We here report the -to our knowledge- first extensive measurement of the occurrence of Sleeping Beauties in the science literature. We derived from the measurements an &#8216;awakening&#8217; probability function and identified the &#8216;most extreme Sleeping Beauty so far&#8217;.

DOI

[22]
van Raan,A.F.J. (2015). Dormitory of physical and engineering sciences: Sleeping Beauties may be sleeping innovations. PLoS ONE, 10(10): e0139786.Abstract A 'Sleeping Beauty in Science' is a publication that goes unnoticed ('sleeps') for a long time and then, almost suddenly, attracts a lot of attention ('is awakened by a prince'). The aim of this paper is to present a general methodology to investigate (1) important properties of Sleeping Beauties such as the time-dependent distribution, author characteristics, journals and fields, and (2) the cognitive environment of Sleeping Beauties. We are particularly interested to find out to what extent Sleeping Beauties are application-oriented and thus are potential Sleeping Innovations. In this study we focus primarily on physics (including materials science and astrophysics) and present first results for chemistry and for engineering & computer science. We find that more than half of the SBs are application-oriented. To study the cognitive environments of Sleeping Beauties we develop a new approach in which the cognitive environment of the SBs is analyzed, based on the mapping of Sleeping Beauties using their citation links and conceptual relations, particularly co-citation mapping. In this way we investigate the research themes in which the SBs are 'used' and possible causes of why the premature work in the SBs becomes topical, i.e., the trigger of the awakening of the SBs. This approach is tested with a blue skies SB and an application-oriented SB. We think that the mapping procedures discussed in this paper are not only important for bibliometric analyses. They also provide researchers with useful, interactive tools to discover both relevant older work as well as new developments, for instance in themes related to Sleeping Beauties that are also Sleeping Innovations.

DOI PMID

[23]
van Raan, A.F.J. (2017). Sleeping beauties cited in patents: Is there also a dormitory of inventions? Scientometrics, 110(3), 1123-1156.A ‘Sleeping Beauty in Science’ is a publication that goes unnoticed (‘sleeps’) for a long time and then, almost suddenly, attracts a lot of attention (‘is awakened by a prince’). In our foregoing study we found that roughly half of the Sleeping Beauties are application-oriented and thus are potential Sleeping Innovations. In this paper we investigate a new topic: Sleeping Beauties that are cited in patents. In this way we explore the existence of a dormitory of inventions. To our knowledge this is the first study of this kind. We investigate the time lag between publication of the Sleeping Beauty and the first citation by a patent. We find that patent citation may occur before or after the awakening and that the depth of the sleep, i.e., citation rate during the sleeping period, is no predictor for later scientific or technological impact of the Sleeping Beauty. A surprising finding is that Sleeping Beauties are significantly more cited in patents than ‘normal’ papers. Inventor–author self-citations relations occur only in a small minority of the Sleeping Beauties that are cited in patents, but other types of inventor–author links occur more frequently. We develop an approach in different steps to explore the cognitive environment of Sleeping Beauties cited in patents. First, we analyze whether they deal with new topics by measuring the time-dependent evolution in the entire scientific literature of the number of papers related to both the precisely defined topics as well as the broader research theme of the Sleeping Beauty during and after the sleeping time. Second, we focus on the awakening by analyzing the first group of papers that cites the Sleeping Beauty. Third, we create concept maps of the topic-related and the citing papers for a time period immediately following the awakening and for the most recent period. Finally, we make an extensive assessment of the cited and citing relations of the Sleeping Beauty. We find that tunable co-citation analysis is a powerful tool to discover the prince(s) and other important application-oriented work directly related to the Sleeping Beauty, for instance papers written by authors who cite Sleeping Beauties in both the patents of which they are the inventors, as well as in their scientific papers.

DOI PMID

[24]
Ye F.Y., &Bornmann ,L.(2018). “Smart girls” versus “sleeping beauties” in the sciences: The identification of instant and delayed recognition by using the citation angle. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 69(3), 359-367.In recent years, a number of studies have introduced methods for identifying papers with delayed recognition (so called 0904sleeping beauties,09 SBs) or have presented single publications as cases of SBs. Most recently, Ke, Ferrara, Radicchi, and Flammini (, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 112(24), 742609“7431) proposed the so called 0904beauty coefficient09 (denoted as B) to quantify how much a given paper can be considered as a paper with delayed recognition. In this study, the new term smart girl (SG) is suggested to differentiate instant credit or 0904flashes in the pan09 from SBs. Although SG and SB are qualitatively defined, the dynamic citation angle 0205 is introduced in this study as a simple way for identifying SGs and SBs quantitatively 09” complementing the beauty coefficient B. The citation angles for all articles from 1980 (n09‰=09‰166,870) in natural sciences are calculated for identifying SGs and SBs and their extent. We reveal that about 3% of the articles are typical SGs and about 0.1% typical SBs. The potential advantages of the citation angle approach are explained.

DOI

[25]
Zirkle C. (1964). Some oddities in the delayed discovery of Mendelism. Journal of Heredity, 55(2), 65-72.

Outlines

/

京ICP备05002861号-43

Copyright © 2023 All rights reserved Journal of Data and Information Science

E-mail: jdis@mail.las.ac.cn Add:No.33, Beisihuan Xilu, Haidian District, Beijing 100190, China

Support by Beijing Magtech Co.ltd E-mail: support@magtech.com.cn